|
re:只说一下这个:“而FC除了个别人其他都被...
只说一下这个:“而FC除了个别人其他都被证明不行” -- 这事关研究方法上的路线斗争。
这儿有个支持 FC 有效性的研究单子,有兴趣的话可以去看:
Studies Supporting Authorship:
Broderick, A. A. & Kasa-Hendrickson, C. (2001). “Say just one word at first”: the emergence of reliable speech in a student labeled with autism. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 26, 13-24.
“This article presents a qualitative, interpretivist research study that documents the emergence, in the context of typed expression, of increasingly useful and reliable speech for a young person labeled with autism (13).
“Jamie’s experience presents a challenge to us as researchers, theoreticians, and educators to broaden the theoretical and conceptual frameworks that we use in order to account for the complexity of this young man’s experience. We are challenged to account for Jamie’s experience by understanding it not as a model, nor as an exceptional or anomalous case, but as a vision of possibility that may illuminate the experiences of others whose language development falls outside of our current conceptual models (23).
Calculator, S. & Singer, K. (1992). Preliminary validation of facilitated communication.
Topics in Language Disorders, 12, ix-xvi.
“The purpose of this study was to validate the impact of facilitated communication on uncovering students’ underlying communication skills…Thus, this study was undertaken with the intention of providing accurate diagnostic information on students as a basis for designing appropriate educational programs (xi).
Cardinal, D. N., Hanson, D. & Wakeham, J. (1996). Investigation of authorship in
facilitated communication. Mental Retardation, 34, 231-242.
“There were two main findings of the study. First, under controlled conditions, some facilitated communication users can pass information to a facilitator when that facilitator is not aware of the information, and second, the measurement of facilitated communication under test conditions may be significantly benefited by extensive practice of the test protocol. This latter result could partially account for the inability of several past studies to verify facilitated communication-user originated input (238).
Emerson, A., Grayson, A., & Griffiths, A. (2001). Can’t or won’t? Evidence relating to
authorship in facilitated communication. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36 (Supp), 98-103.
“Data for 14 of the participants who have been introduced to FC is included in this paper…The summarized data relate to the issue of ‘authorship’, i.e. the question of which of the communication partners (facilitator or user) is really responsible for the emergent text. The data come from two main sources- controlled tests (in the style of published experimental studies) and transcripts or diary records of routinely occurring FC sessions (99).
“Evidence from this project shows similar findings to many of the published studies that conclude, having undertaken controlled tests, that FC is not a valid strategy to use. However, evidence from the same project also suggests that the overall picture with regard to FC may be more complex than this. The same participants who do not provide authorship evidence in controlled trials provide data which indicate that they are authoring their communications when given the opportunity to communicate about things of their own choosing (100).
Janzen-Wilde, M., Duchan, J., & Higginbotham, D. (1995). Successful use of facilitated
communication with an oral child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 658-676.
“A 6 ½-year-old child’s oral and spelled utterances were compared over a 3-month period as he was trained to use Facilitated Communication (FC), a method of augmentative and alternative communication. The child’s language with FC was significantly better than his oral language in length of utterances, novelty of utterances, and syntactic complexity. His language with FC also contained more function words and over time was more intelligible and required less verbal scaffolding than his oral communication. Evidence that he was authoring his own messages during his facilitated spelling was found in his idiosyncratic use of language and his ability to convey verifiable information that was unknown to the facilitator (658).
“Andy’s introduction to FC allowed people to see areas of his competence that were previously unknown to those around him. It is hoped that this study will promote further research to increase understanding of FC and its role in language intervention. Such understanding can only serve to benefit individuals like Andy, who have much to say (671).
Niemi, J. & Karna-Lin, E. (2002). Grammar and lexicon in facilitated communication: A
linguistic authorship analysis of a Finnish case. Mental Retardation, 40, 347-357.
“This case study adds a new dimension to the discourse on the authorship issue in facilitated communication. The linguistic structure produced by a young Finnish man with severe cerebral palsy was examined. Data are based on transcripts he produced from 1993 until 1996 after facilitated communication had been introduced to him. In the data analysis, as explicit criteria for his idiosyncrasies, we used patterns typical of children acquiring Finnish as their first language and those found in normal slips of the tongue, acquired aphasia, and specific language impairment. Based on the analysis (i.e. the idiosyncrasy and agrammaticality of word-forms and sentences), we strongly suggest that his output can hardly be a product of any other speaker of Finnish, including that of his facilitators (347).
“Even though this study has a limited amount of data from a person using facilitated communication, our results provide strong evidence for the claim that the text produced by facilitation originates from the author, not from the facilitator (355).
Rubin, S., Biklen, D., Kasa-Hendrickson, C., Kluth, P., Cardinal, D., & Broderick, A.
(2001). Independence, participation, and the meaning of intellectual ability. Disability & Society, 16, 415-429.
“This article presents a non-speaking person’s perspectives on independence and the implications of newfound communication abilities for her participation in the world and upon the meaning of intellectual ability. The person with the communication disability also has autism and, early in her life, was classified by school officials as ‘severely retarded.’ The narrative focuses especially on the concepts of independence, participation, and intellectual competence or intellectual performance, and their relationship to the concepts of democracy, freedom, and identity, all from a non-essentialist perspective. In addition, the article addresses practical questions about how, from her perspective, the non-speaking person developed the ability to communicate without physical support (415).
“Rubin: Because of the way we move and our lack of speech we were assumed to be retarded. I was thought to be retarded (but) all this changed…once I could type without support…My very existence challenged beliefs about mental retardation. Able to type independently…my presentations (at conferences) were acts of advocacy (419).
Sheehan, C. & Matuozzi, R. (1996). Investigation of the validity of facilitated
communication through the disclosure of unknown information. Mental Retardation, 34, 94-107.
“Three individuals (8, 10, and 24 years old with diagnoses of autism and mental retardation) participated in a message-passing format to determine whether they could disclose information previously unknown to their facilitators. Results reveal valid facilitated communication from each participant (94).
“The data from the current study lead us to caution that a phenomena as complex as facilitated communication eludes a cursory exploration. Each participant was able to disclose information accurately and deftly at times and was wholly inadequate in his or her attempts at other times…The developing picture of an individual’s validity profile replete with the patterns of required support, inconsistency, language impairment, and strides towards independence may well be the only reasonable evaluation of a validity confidence level (104).”
Tuzzi, A., Cemin, M. Castagna, M. (2004) “Moved deeply I am” Autistic language in texts produced with FC. Journees internationals d’Analyse statistique des Donnees Textuelleds, 7, 1-9.
“Using texts produced through Facilitated Communication (FC), this work is aimed at identifying the characteristic features of the language used by autistic subjects and understanding when these distinctive elements may distinguish it from the language of facilitators. Preliminary results shows that autistic subjects actually use a special style of writing; this finding supports the hypothesis that texts are the fruit of individual production of autistic subjects, not inevitably influenced by facilitators. This first work, based on a restricted sample which is not necessarily representative, is important because it has permitted to better specify criteria by which subjects, texts, analyses to carry out and software to be employed will be chosen in future studies.” (1)
Weiss, M., Wagner, S., & Bauman, M. (1996). A validated case study of facilitated
communication. Mental Retardation, 34, 220-230.
“The case of a 13-year-old boy with autism, severe mental retardation, and a seizure disorder who was able to demonstrate valid facilitated communication is described (220).
It is tempting to offer conjecture about why this procedure or these individuals were able to reveal valid communication with facilitated communication. However, we find it far too premature to draw any such conclusions. Rather, we have chosen to highlight for consideration a small number of factors that may have been relevant (227).
Zanobini, M. & Scopesi, A. (2001). La comunicazione facilitata in un bambino autistico.
Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 5, 395-421.
“Studied facilitated communicative interactions among a 7-yr-old autistic boy, his mother, and his teacher. Data on communication was obtained by observation of 28 interactions in natural situations. The Ss' verbal productions were transcribed and evaluated according to context and situation, stylistic consistency, and interaction with mother or teacher.
“The Sphinx Lexica Edition software program (1998) was used. The results show evidence of stylistic consistency but variation according to location at home or school and interaction with mother or teacher. The results suggest that the boy's original and peculiar linguistic behavior may indicate a degree of linguistic independence from facilitation. Implications for improving facilitated communication for autistic children are discussed.”
|
|