以琳自闭症论坛

 找回密码
 注册 (请写明注册原因,12小时内通过审核)
查看: 3278|回复: 2
打印 上一主题 下一主题

Autism’s First Child(转载)

[复制链接]
跳转到指定楼层
1#
发表于 2010-10-29 10:53:18 | 只看该作者 回帖奖励 |倒序浏览 |阅读模式
http://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2010/10/autism-8217-s-first-child/8227/

As new cases of autism have exploded in recent years—some form of the condition affects about one in 110 children today—efforts have multiplied to understand and accommodate the condition in childhood. But children with autism will become adults with autism, some 500,000 of them in this decade alone. What then? Meet Donald Gray Triplett, 77, of Forest, Mississippi. He was the first person ever diagnosed with autism. And his long, happy, surprising life may hold some answers.

By John Donvan and Caren Zucker
(John Donvan [Donald Number 550] is a correspondent for ABC’s Nightline. Caren Zucker [Donald Number 549] is a television producer and the mother of a teenager with autism. They’ve been collecting forgotten stories of autism for a book. )


In 1951, a Hungarian-born psychologist, mind reader, and hypnotist named Franz Polgar was booked for a single night’s performance in a town called Forest, Mississippi, at the time a community of some 3,000 people and no hotel accommodations. Perhaps because of his social position—he went by Dr. Polgar, had appeared in Life magazine, and claimed (falsely) to have been Sigmund Freud’s “medical hypnotist”—Polgar was lodged at the home of one of Forest’s wealthiest and best-educated couples, who treated the esteemed mentalist as their personal guest.

Polgar’s all-knowing, all-seeing act had been mesmerizing audiences in American towns large and small for several years. But that night it was his turn to be dazzled, when he met the couple’s older son, Donald, who was then 18. Oddly distant, uninterested in conversation, and awkward in his movements, Donald nevertheless possessed a few advanced faculties of his own, including a flawless ability to name musical notes as they were played on a piano and a genius for multiplying numbers in his head. Polgar tossed out “87 times 23,” and Donald, with his eyes closed and not a hint of hesitation, correctly answered “2,001.”

Indeed, Donald was something of a local legend. Even people in neighboring towns had heard of the Forest teenager who’d calculated the number of bricks in the facade of the high school—the very building in which Polgar would be performing—merely by glancing at it.

According to family lore, Polgar put on his show and then, after taking his final bows, approached his hosts with a proposal: that they let him bring Donald with him on the road, as part of his act.

Donald’s parents were taken aback. “My mother,” recalls Donald’s brother, Oliver, “was not at all interested.” For one, things were finally going well for Donald, after a difficult start in life. “She explained to [Polgar] that he was in school, he had to keep going to classes,” Oliver says. He couldn’t simply drop everything for a run at show business, especially not when he had college in his sights.

But there was also, whether they spoke this aloud to their guest or not, the sheer indignity of what Polgar was proposing. Donald’s being odd, his parents could not undo; his being made an oddity of, they could, and would, prevent. The offer was politely but firmly declined.

What the all-knowing mentalist didn’t know, however, was that Donald, the boy who missed the chance to share his limelight, already owned a place in history. His unusual gifts and deficits had been noted outside Mississippi, and an account of them had been published—one that was destined to be translated and reprinted all over the world, making his name far better-known, in time, than Polgar’s.

His first name, anyway.

Donald was the first child ever diagnosed with autism. Identified in the annals of autism as “Case 1 … Donald T,” he is the initial subject described in a 1943 medical article that announced the discovery of a condition unlike “anything reported so far,” the complex neurological ailment now most often called an autism spectrum disorder, or ASD. At the time, the condition was considered exceedingly rare, limited to Donald and 10 other children—Cases 2 through 11—also cited in that first article.

That was 67 years ago. Today, physicians, parents, and politicians regularly speak of an “epidemic” of autism. The rate of ASDs, which come in a range of forms and widely varying degrees of severity—hence spectrum—has been accelerating dramatically since the early 1990s, and some form of ASD is now estimated to affect one in every 110 American children. And nobody knows why.

There have always been theories about the cause of autism—many theories. In the earliest days, it was an article of faith among psychiatrists that autism was brought on by bad mothers, whose chilly behavior toward their children led the youngsters to withdraw into a safe but private world. In time, autism was recognized to have a biological basis. But this understanding, rather than producing clarity, instead unleashed a contentious debate about the exact mechanisms at work. Differing factions argue that the gluten in food causes autism; that the mercury used as a preservative in some vaccines can trigger autistic symptoms; and that the particular measles-mumps-rubella vaccine is to blame. Other schools of thought have portrayed autism as essentially an autoimmune response, or the result of a nutritional deficiency. The mainstream consensus today—that autism is a neurological condition probably resulting from one or more genetic abnormalities in combination with an environmental trigger—offers little more in the way of explanation: the number of genes and triggers that could be involved is so large that a definitive cause, much less a cure, is unlikely to be determined anytime soon. Even the notion that autism cases are on the rise is disputed to a degree, with some believing that the escalating diagnoses largely result from a greater awareness of what autism looks like.

There is no longer much dispute, however, about the broad outlines of what constitutes a case of autism. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—the so-called bible of psychiatry—draws a clear map of symptoms. And to a remarkable degree, these symptoms still align with those of one “Donald T,” who was first examined at Johns Hopkins University, in Baltimore, in the 1930s, the same boy who would later amaze a mentalist and become renowned for counting bricks.

In subsequent years, the scientific literature updated Donald T’s story a few times, a journal entry here or there, but about four decades ago, that narrative petered out. The later chapters in his life remained unwritten, leaving us with no detailed answer to the question Whatever happened to Donald?

There is an answer. Some of it we turned up in documents long overlooked in the archives of Johns Hopkins. But most of it we found by tracking down and spending time with Donald himself. His full name is Donald Gray Triplett. He’s 77 years old. And he’s still in Forest, Mississippi. Playing golf.

The question that haunts every parent of a child with autism is What will happen when I die? This reflects a chronological inevitability: children with autism will grow up to become adults with autism, in most cases ultimately outliving the parents who provided their primary support.

Then what?

It’s a question that has yet to grab society’s attention, as the discussion of autism to date has skewed, understandably, toward its impact on childhood. But the stark fact is that an epidemic among children today means an epidemic among adults tomorrow. The statistics are dramatic: within a decade or so, more than 500,000 children diagnosed with autism will enter adulthood. Some of them will have the less severe variants—Asperger’s syndrome or HFA, which stands for “high-functioning autism”—and may be able to live more independent and fulfilling lives. But even that subgroup will require some support, and the needs of those with lower-functioning varieties of autism will be profound and constant.

How we respond to those needs will be shaped in great measure by how we choose to view adults with autism. We can dissociate from them, regarding them as tragically broken persons, and hope we are humane enough to shoulder the burden of meeting their basic needs. This is the view that sees the disabled in general as wards of the community, morally and perhaps legally, and that, in the relatively recent past, often “solved” the “problem” of these disabled adults by warehousing them for life—literally in wards.

Alternatively, we can dispense with the layers of sorrow, and interpret autism as but one more wrinkle in the fabric of humanity. Practically speaking, this does not mean pretending that adults with autism do not need help. But it does mean replacing pity toward them with ambition for them. The key to this view is a recognition that “they” are part of “us,” so that those who don’t have autism are actively rooting for those who do.

Donald Triplett, the first person cast in the story of autism, has spent time in the worlds shaped by each of these views.

Donald drives his car with a light, percussive rhythm. After pressing on the gas pedal for a second, he lets up briefly, and then presses back down again. Down. Release. Down. Release. The tempo doesn’t vary. It’s late afternoon, and Donald is guiding his coffee-colored 2000 Cadillac, in hardly perceptible surges and glides, south along Mississippi’s Route 80. Though his forward posture and two-fisted grip on the wheel are those of an old man, his face beams like a boy’s. He wears the expression, at once relaxed and resolute, of a man who is doing precisely what he wants to be doing.

The day’s agenda thus far has included morning coffee with friends, a long walk for exercise, a Bonanza rerun on TV, and now, at 4:30, this short drive down Route 80 to get in some golf. “I noticed,” he mentions, “you have a Lafayette County sticker on your car.” He’s broken a long silence with that comment, a reference to the registration decal on the rental we parked in his driveway. His words hang there for a moment, and then he adds: “That means it comes from Lafayette County.” That’s all. Nodding to himself, Donald goes silent again, his focus returning to the road ahead, or tuned to some inner monologue. Given his tendency to close his eyes for long moments when he speaks, this is probably the safest choice.

He parks just short of the front steps of the Forest Country Club, an establishment without pretensions. The one-story red-brick clubhouse fronts onto a well-tended, mostly flat course carved out of the woods. Membership is $100 per family per month, and a round of 18 holes costs $20 on a weekday. On any given day, the roster of players on the fairways includes lawyers and mechanics, bankers and truckers, salesmen and farmers—and Donald. Actually, Donald is there every day, weather permitting. And almost every day, he golfs alone.

Not everyone who plays here realizes that “DT”—as he’s known around the club—has autism. But his quirks are hard to miss as he makes his way to the first tee, well within sight of members who take the shade in armchairs under the club’s columned portico. A small man in khaki shorts and a green knit shirt, with a pink-camouflage bucket hat pulled down tight over his ears, Donald strides to the tee with the distinctive gait that is often a tip-off for autism—his arms out from his sides in the shape of a large capital A, his steps just slightly mechanical, his head and shoulders bobbing left-right-left in the rocking movement of a metronome.

The fact is that Donald’s not a bad golfer: tee shots mostly on the fairway, passable short game, can nail a six-foot putt. His swing, however, is an unfolding pantomime, a ritual of gestures he seems compelled to repeat with almost every shot—especially when he really wants the ball to travel.

He licks the fingers of his right hand, and then his left. Squaring himself to the ball, he raises his club skyward, until it’s straight up over his head, as if he were hoisting a banner. Sometimes he holds his arms up there for a long moment. Then he brings the club head back to earth, stopping not far from the ball, before taking it back up. He goes through a series of these backswings, picking up speed with each iteration until, stiff-legged, he inches forward to get his head over the ball. With one final stroke, he commits to contact. Crack! It’s gone, and Donald, bouncing up and down at the knees, peers down the fairway to see the result. As a swing, it’s the opposite of fluid. But it’s Donald’s own. And he never whiffs it.

Some days, Donald has no choice but to partner with other golfers, when the country club, honoring golf’s traditions as a social game, reserves the entire course for a membership “scramble.” In a scramble, golfers are randomly assigned to teams, which compete for lowest group score by picking the ball in the best position and having everyone on the team play from that spot. During one recent scramble, Donald made the rounds with Lori and Elk and Kenneth and Mary, all of whom seemed to be at least three or four decades younger than he was. But Donald held his own competitively, with his shots often enough the ones used. He also kicked in a passable amount of friendly banter, which was returned in the same spirit, though Donald’s patter tended to get repetitive: “Way to hit that ball, Kenneth!” “Way to hit that ball, Lori!” “Way to hit that ball, Elk!” At times he would entertain variations, marrying his partners’ names with words from his own private vocabulary: “Hey, Elkins the Elk!” “Hey, Mary Cherry!” “Okay, thank you, Kenneth the Senneth!”

Most of the time, however, Donald remained silent. This is in keeping with the decorum of the game, of course. But Donald appears comfortable with silence, and in a larger sense, content with the life he’s leading, which resembles—with the car and the coffee and the golf and the TV—a retirement community’s brochure version of how to live out the golden years. Donald has freedom, independence, and good health. All in all, life has turned out well for autism’s first child.

Donald was institutionalized when he was only 3 years old. Records in the archives at Johns Hopkins quote the family doctor in Mississippi suggesting that the Tripletts had “overstimulated the child.” Donald’s refusal as a toddler to feed himself, combined with other problem behaviors his parents could not handle, prompted the doctor’s recommendation for “a change of environment.” In August 1937, Donald entered a state-run facility 50 miles from his home, in a town then actually called Sanatorium, Mississippi.

The large building where he was housed served what today seems an odd function: preemptive isolation for children thought to be at risk of catching tuberculosis. The place wasn’t designed or operated with a child like Donald in mind, and according to a medical evaluator, his response upon arrival was dramatic: he “faded away physically.”

At the time, institutionalization was the default option for severe mental illness, which even his mother believed was at the root of Donald’s behavior: she described him in one despairing letter as her “hopelessly insane child.” Being in an institution, however, didn’t help. “It seems,” his Johns Hopkins evaluator later wrote, “he had there his worst phase.” With parental visits limited to twice a month, his predisposition to avoid contact with people broadened to everything else—toys, food, music, movement—to the point where daily he “sat motionless, paying no attention to anything.”

He had not been diagnosed correctly, of course, because the correct diagnosis did not yet exist. Very likely he was not alone in that sense, and there were other children with autism, in other wards in other states, similarly misdiagnosed—perhaps as “feeble-minded,” in the medical parlance of the day, or more likely, because of the strong but isolated intelligence skills many could demonstrate, as having schizophrenia.

Donald’s parents came for him in August of 1938. By then, at the end of a year of institutionalization, Donald was eating again, and his health had returned. Though he now “played among the other children,” his observers noted, he did so “without taking part in their occupations.” The facility’s director nonetheless told Donald’s parents that the boy was “getting along nicely,” and tried to talk them out of removing their son. He actually requested that they “let him alone.”

But they held their ground, and took Donald home with them. Later, when they asked the director to provide them with a written assessment of Donald’s time there, he could scarcely be bothered. His remarks on Donald’s full year under his care covered less than half a page. The boy’s problem, he concluded, was probably “some glandular disease.”

Donald, about to turn 5 years old, was back where he had started.

Dr. Peter Gerhardt waves a credit card in his right hand, animated—as he often is—about the point he’s trying to make. This time, it’s a trick that he guarantees makes it impossible to swipe the card the wrong way. “You can slide it this way, or like this, or stick it in like this”—he jabs straight into the air in front of him, as if into a bank machine—“and if you keep your thumb in this position on the card, you will always swipe it the correct way.”

Closer examination clarifies: the card he is holding is a Visa, and his right thumb completely covers the blue-on-white logo in the lower right corner, the sweet spot that makes the trick work. Keep your thumb there, Gerhardt pronounces, and the magnetic stripe will always line up properly, regardless of the type of card reader.

Gerhardt’s demonstration isn’t intended merely for news-you-can-use convenience. Rather, he’s explaining how using a bank card fits into the bigger picture that has defined his career since he embarked on his doctorate in educational psychology at Rutgers in the early 1980s: the struggle people with autism face to be accepted into a world occupied by “the rest of us.”

The truth is that we often deny to adults with autism the kind of empathy and support we make readily available to children with the condition—or, for that matter, to people with white canes at crosswalks. We underestimate their capabilities, reveal our discomfort in their company, and display impatience when they inconvenience us. The people standing in the back of a long supermarket checkout line aren’t always going to say or do the nice thing when some odd-looking man in front is holding the whole place up because he can’t figure out the credit-card swipe. It’s in that moment, Gerhardt says, that the thumb-on-the-logo trick is a matter of “social survival.” If the man with autism can navigate this situation successfully—and, just as important, be seen doing so—Gerhardt argues that our collective acceptance of people with autism in “our” spaces will tick up a notch. If the man fails, it will go the other way.

Gerhardt, who is a former president of the Virginia-based Organization for Autism Research and is now developing a program focused on adolescence to adulthood at the respected McCarton School in New York, is considered among the top experts in the country working with adults who have autism. But he jokes that this is chiefly because he’s never faced much competition. “I have an entire career,” he says, “based on people not wanting my job.” Child development is the hot area in autism research; working with adults, Gerhardt says, “is not a career move.” Adults present greater challenges: they are big enough to do real violence in the event of a tantrum; they are fully capable of sexual desires, and all that those imply; and they’re bored by many of the activities that can distract and entertain children with autism. “People want to treat these adults like little kids in big bodies,” Gerhardt says. “They can’t. They’re adults.” As such, he argues, they’re equipped, as much as any of us, with the recognizable adult aspiration of wanting to “experience life.”

“It’s having friends,” Gerhardt explains. “It’s having interesting work. It’s having something you want. It’s all the things the rest of us value, once given an opportunity.”

Gerhardt wants priority given to teaching the kinds of skills adults with autism need in order to survive independently: keeping track of money, asking for directions and then following them, wearing clean clothes, navigating public transport, recognizing a dangerous person, and—of extreme difficulty for most—looking a job interviewer in the eye. Gerhardt disputes the doubts he hears even within his profession about encouraging adults with autism to aspire to independence. “What’s the worst thing that can happen?” he asks. “You know—he’s at the supermarket and he drops some eggs, or somebody thinks he’s a little weird. I would rather he be there alone, and only getting nine out of 10 items he came shopping for, than need me there with him to get all 10. That’s a much better way to live.”

This leads to the question of where they will live. As it is, 85 percent of adults with autism still live with parents, siblings, or other relatives. But what happens when that is no longer an option? Large-scale warehousing is gone—and good riddance, most say. An obvious alternative is residential arrangements offering multiple spaces to people with autism, who can share support services under one roof in a setting that really is a home. At present, however, given both start-up costs and resistance from neighbors, the number of spaces in such homes is limited, and landing a spot can be extremely difficult: nationally, more than 88,000 adults are already on waiting lists.

All of which leads to an unsettling answer for those parents asking what happens, after they die, to their children with autism. We don’t really know.

Most likely, Donald’s name would never have entered the medical literature had his parents not had both the ambition to seek out the best help for him, and the resources to pay for it. Mary Triplett had been born into the McCravey family, financiers who had founded and still controlled the Bank of Forest. Uncommonly for a woman at that time, particularly in that milieu, she had a college degree. After a doomed romance with a local cotton farmer’s son, whom her family forbade her to marry—he later went on to renown as six-term segregationist U.S. Senator James “Big Jim” Eastland—she instead married the former mayor’s son, an attorney named Oliver Triplett Jr. With a degree from Yale Law School and a private practice located directly opposite the county courthouse, Oliver would later hold the position of Forest town attorney and would be admitted to the bar of the Supreme Court of the United States. He was an intense man who had suffered two nervous breakdowns, and who could get so lost in his thoughts that he’d return from walks in town with no recollection of having seen anyone or anything along the way. But as a lawyer, he was considered brilliant, and when he proposed to Mary, her family apparently raised no objections.

Their first son, Donald, was born in September 1933. A brother came along nearly five years later, while Donald was in Sanatorium. Also named Oliver, the baby stayed behind with his grandparents in Forest when, in October 1938, the rest of the family boarded a Pullman car in Meridian, Mississippi, headed for Baltimore. Donald’s parents had secured him a consultation with the nation’s top child psychiatrist at the time, a Johns Hopkins professor named Dr. Leo Kanner.
2#
 楼主| 发表于 2010-10-29 10:53:46 | 只看该作者

re:Kanner (pronounced “...

Kanner (pronounced “Connor”) had written the book, literally, on child psychiatry. Aptly titled Child Psychiatry, this definitive 1935 work immediately became the standard medical-school text, and was reprinted through 1972. No doubt Kanner’s stature was enhanced by his pedigree—he was an Austrian Jew with a medical degree from the University of Berlin—while his nearly impenetrable accent perfectly fit the image Americans had in mind when they used the word psychiatrist.

Kanner would always seem slightly perplexed by the intensity of the letter he had received from Donald’s father in advance of their meeting. Before departing Mississippi, Oliver had retreated to his law office and dictated a detailed medical and psychological history covering the first five years of his elder son’s life. Typed up by his secretary and sent ahead to Kanner, it came to 33 pages. Many times over the years, Kanner would refer to the letter’s “obsessive detail.”

Excerpts from Oliver’s letter—the outpourings of a layman, but also a parent—now hold a unique place in the canon of autism studies. Cited for decades and translated into several languages, Oliver’s observations were the first detailed listing of symptoms that are now instantly recognizable to anyone who knows autism. It is not too much to say that the agreed-upon diagnosis of autism—the one being applied today to define an epidemic—was modeled, at least in part, on Donald’s symptoms as described by his father.

Their little boy, Oliver wrote, had almost never cried to be with his mother. He appeared to have withdrawn “into his shell,” to “live within himself,” to be “perfectly oblivious to everything about him.” Entirely uninterested in human beings—including his parents, for whom he displayed “no apparent affection”—he nevertheless had several obsessions, including “a mania for spinning blocks and pans and other round objects.” He was fascinated with numbers, musical notes, pictures of U.S. presidents, and the letters of the alphabet, which he enjoyed reciting in reverse order.

Physically awkward, he also had intense dislikes: milk, swings, tricycles—“almost a horror of them”—and any change in routine or interruption of his internal thought processes: “When interfered with he has temper tantrums, during which he is destructive.” Generally nonresponsive when his name was called—he seemed not to have heard—he instead had “to be picked up and carried or led wherever he ought to go.” When asked a question, if he answered at all, he generally kept his response to one word, and then only if it derived from something he had memorized. Certain words and phrases captivated him, and he would loop them aloud endlessly: trumpet vine, business, chrysanthemum.

At the same time, Donald exhibited some prodigious, if isolated, mental skills. By the age of 2, he could recite the 23rd Psalm (“Yea though I walk through the valley of the shadow of death …”) and knew 25 questions and answers from the Presbyterian catechism by heart. And the random humming he engaged in while spinning blocks turned out not to be quite so random after all. Rather, he always picked three notes that, if played simultaneously on a keyboard, would blend into a perfect chord. Alone in thought, Donald gave the impression of a quite intelligent little boy, working through some sort of problem. “He appears to be always thinking and thinking,” his father wrote. He was, in a heartrendingly comprehensive phrase, “happiest when left alone.”

When Kanner finally met Donald, he confirmed all this, and more. Donald entered the room, Kanner later recalled, and headed straight for the blocks and toys, “without paying the least attention to the persons present.” Kanner had a trick up his sleeve that today would draw disapproval: he pricked Donald with a pin. The result was revealing. Donald didn’t like it—it hurt—but he didn’t like Kanner any less for doing it. To Kanner, it seemed that he could not attach the pain to the person who’d inflicted it. Throughout the visit, in fact, Donald remained completely indifferent to Kanner, as uninterested in him as in “the desk, the bookshelf, or the filing cabinet.”

The surviving medical records of that initial visit contain a notation preceded by a question mark: schizophrenia. It was one of the few diagnoses that came even close to making sense, because it was clear that Donald was essentially an intelligent child, as a person exhibiting schizophrenia might easily be. But nothing in his behavior suggested that Donald experienced the hallucinations typical of schizophrenia. He wasn’t seeing things that weren’t there, even if he was ignoring the people who were.

Kanner kept Donald under observation for two weeks, and then the Tripletts returned to Mississippi—without answers. Kanner simply had no idea how to diagnose the child. He would later write to Mary Triplett, who had begun sending frequent updates on Donald: “Nobody realizes more than I do myself that at no time have you or your husband been given a clear-cut and unequivocal … diagnostic term.” It was dawning on him, he wrote, that he was seeing “for the first time a condition which has not hitherto been described by psychiatric or any other literature.”

He wrote those lines to Mary in a letter dated September 1942, almost four years after he’d first seen Donald. The family had made three follow-up visits to Baltimore, all equally inconclusive. Perhaps hoping to allay her frustration, Kanner added that he was beginning to see a picture emerge. “I have now accumulated,” he wrote, “a series of eight other cases which are very much like Don’s.” He hadn’t gone public with this, he noted, because he needed “time for longer observation.”

He had, however, been working on a name for this new condition. Pulling together the distinctive symptoms exhibited by Donald and the eight other children—their lack of interest in people, their fascination with objects, their need for sameness, their keenness to be left alone—he wrote Mary: “If there is any name to be applied to the condition of Don and those other children, I have found it best to speak of it as ‘autistic disturbance of affective contact.’”

Kanner did not coin the term autistic. It was already in use in psychiatry, not as the name of a syndrome but as an observational term describing the way some patients with schizophrenia withdrew from contact with those around them. Like the word feverish, it described a symptom, not an illness. But now Kanner was using it to pinpoint and label a complex set of behaviors that together constituted a single, never-before-recognized diagnosis: autism. (As it happens, another Austrian, Hans Asperger, was working at the same time in Vienna with children who shared some similar characteristics, and independently applied the identical word—autistic to the behaviors he was seeing; his paper on the subject would come out a year after Kanner’s, but remained largely unknown until it was translated into English in the early 1990s.)

Kanner published his findings in 1943, in a journal called The Nervous Child. Since writing to Mary the previous year, he had added two more cases to this total: 11 children, 11 histories. But he started the story with Donald.

For all the progress that Donald has made in the decades since—the driving, the golfing—conversation is an art that continues to elude him. He initiates on occasion, but his purpose is generally to elicit a piece of information he needs (“What time is lunch?”) or to make a passing observation (his comment about the sticker on our car). A regular chat, the casual back-and-forth of kicking around an idea, is something he has never experienced.

When asked questions—even questions that invite some elaboration—he responds in a terse, one-way manner, like a man working his way through a questionnaire.

Topic: Donald’s sense of achievement at being able to multiply in his head

“Donald, how does it make you feel that it just comes out of your head?”

“It just comes out.”

“Does it make you feel good?”

“Oh yes, oh yes.”

“Can you describe it?”

“No, I can’t describe it.”

Topic: Donald’s memory of meeting the mentalist Franz Polgar

“Donald, do you remember Franz Polgar?”

“Yes, I do remember Franz Polgar.”

[Silence.]

“When did he come?”

“Actually he came twice. He came in 1950 and 1951.”

[Another long lapse.]

“Who was he?”

“He was a hypnotist.”

“Can you tell me what he was like? Was he an old man?”

“He was probably 55 years old. And he’d be 110 if he were living.”

As is clear from these exchanges, Donald’s thinking likes to go to numbers—even when, as in this case, his arithmetic appears faulty—to dates and calculations and constants that order the world concretely and do not require interpretation. He even has a habit of assigning numbers to people he encounters, a sort of internal indexing system. An old acquaintance named Buddy Lovett, who resides one town over, in Morton, Mississippi, told us that Donald had assigned him the number 333 sometime in the late 1950s. Though he had not seen Donald for several years, he urged, with a hint of mischief, “Next time you see him, go ahead: ask him what my number is.”

Indeed, the next day Donald nailed Lovett’s number almost before hearing the end of the question. We ran this test several times, presenting the names of people all over Forest who had told us of being “numbered” over the years. Donald recalled every one, without hang or hiccup, though he can’t explain the underlying system. The numbers just come to him, he says, and then stay forever.

Likewise, those who receive a Donald Number seem to remember it for the rest of their lives. An indelible distinction, a recognition they’ll never have to share—it may feel akin to an honor.

That is almost certainly not what Donald intends. Honor is one of those concepts—an abstraction arbitrating between the ideal and the actual—unlikely to come easily to someone like Donald, who is far more comfortable in a world ordered by established facts, by what literally is. This is why it is generally believed that people with autism have difficulty lying, or appreciating a joke. Although Donald obviously enjoys pondering lists of people, places, and things, he does not engage easily with implication, mood, or emotion.

Topic: The death of his mother, Mary Triplett, who took care of Donald for 52 years

“Donald, when did your mother die?”

“It was 1985. May 1985.”

“Do you remember where you were?”

“I was at the bank. Her doctor had said it was just a matter of time … and I got the word saying that she had passed away with congestive heart failure.”

“Do you remember how you felt?”

“It was rather expected. I wasn’t really downhearted or weeping or anything like that.”

“Were you not downhearted because … ?”

“I just don’t react. Different people react differently to situations like that.”

Asked whether he missed his mother, he replied—questionnaire again—“Yes, I miss her.” He said he also misses his father, whose death in a 1980 car accident he described in a similarly matter-of-fact manner. He recalls that his dad’s accident was a shock and, again, that he didn’t cry.

Peter Gerhardt tells the story of his friend Tony, who was 55 years old when he got a crash course in the condolence hug. Tony, diagnosed with autism as an adult, had lived all his life under the same roof as his mother. Then she died.

The funeral marked the first time in his life that Tony had been placed in the category of “the bereaved,” and, as he mingled among the other funeral-goers, he learned that people in his position must be prepared to accept some intense and lingering hugs. He handled it fine, observing how his brother was responding to the same sorts of approaches, and comprehending that the people doing this were trying to help him not feel sad. Then he went home, hugged his neighbor, and nearly got arrested.

It was the day after the funeral, and the elderly woman who lived next door—not a close family friend, but someone kindly observing the custom of bringing meals when there’s been a death—came to his door with food she’d prepared. Tony thanked her, and she offered condolences.

According to Peter Gerhardt, what happened next is a textbook example of the kind of misunderstanding that bedevils people with autism. “Tony thought, Well, she offered condolences. I’m supposed to hug her. So he went to hug her.” Gerhardt notes that the woman undoubtedly sent off strong social signals that she did not want to be embraced. But Tony failed to pick up on them: “He hugged her, probably somewhat awkwardly—a little too long, a little too hard, a little too low—because she went home and called the police [reporting] a sexual assault by the man next door.”

To Gerhardt, this serves as a parable for interactions between people who have autism and those who don’t: neither party did anything wrong, but neither knew enough to get it right. Tony, a man bright enough to have earned a college degree, simply lacked the instinctive experience—the teachable experience, Gerhardt contends—to tell whether or not a person wants a hug. He was sufficiently self-aware to understand that he was missing vital cues, but he had no idea what they were. He later explained to Gerhardt: “The rules keep changing on me. Every time I think I learn a new rule, you change it on me.”

The answer to this problem, Gerhardt argues, is the right kind of education for the many Tonys out there. At present, he contends, schooling for children with high-functioning levels of autism overemphasizes traditional academic achievement—trying to learn French or the state capitals—at the expense of what someone like Tony really needs, a set of social skills that keep him from making mistakes such as hugging his neighbor the wrong way. These skills—like knowing how to swipe a Visa card—are not generally taught to kids with autism. And once they become adults, the teaching, in all too many cases, stops completely. In general, state-funded education ends the day a person with autism turns 21. Beyond that, there are no legal mandates, and there is very little funding. “It’s like giving someone a wheelchair on a one-month rental,” Gerhardt says, “and at the end of the month, they have to give it back, and walk.”

But there was another side to the equation in the hug incident: the neighbor’s lack of education on the character of autism. Had she been more aware of Tony’s condition, and what it might occasionally entail, she might not have felt so threatened. At the very least, had she understood the situation, she could have simply told Tony that she’d like him to let go, rather than hoping he’d read social cues that were invisible to him.

As it was, the whole situation was quickly defused: Tony’s brother arrived and offered both the neighbor and the police an explanation of Tony’s disability, and she declined to press charges. But, as Gerhardt notes, a little more information on both sides might have prevented this misunderstanding in the first place.

Donald lives alone now, in the house where his parents raised him. Enshrined in honeysuckle and shaded by several old oaks, a few minutes’ walk from Forest’s faded business district, the house needs some paint and repairs. Several of its rooms—including the dining and living rooms, where his parents welcomed visitors—are dark and musty with disuse. Donald rarely enters that part of the house. The kitchen, bathroom, and bedroom are home enough for him.

Except for once a month, that is, when he walks out the front door and leaves town.

Perhaps the most remarkable aspect of Donald’s life is that he grew up to be an avid traveler. He has been to Germany, Tunisia, Hungary, Dubai, Spain, Portugal, France, Bulgaria, and Colombia—some 36 foreign countries and 28 U.S. states in all, including Egypt three times, Istanbul five times, and Hawaii 17. He’s notched one African safari, several cruises, and innumerable PGA tournaments.

It’s not wanderlust exactly. Most times, he sets six days as his maximum time away, and maintains no contact afterward with people he meets along the way. He makes it a mission to get his own snapshots of places he’s already seen in pictures, and assembles them into albums when he gets home. Then he gets to work planning his next foray, calling the airlines himself for domestic travel, and relying on a travel agent in Jackson when he’s going overseas. He is, in all likelihood, the best-traveled man in Forest, Mississippi.

This is the same man whose favorite pastimes, as a boy, were spinning objects, spinning himself, and rolling nonsense words around in his mouth. At the time, he seemed destined for a cramped, barren adulthood—possibly lived out behind the windows of a state institution. Instead, he learned to golf, to drive, and to circumnavigate the globe—skills he first developed at the respective ages of 23, 27, and 36. In adulthood, Donald continued to branch out.

Autism is a highly individualized condition. The amount of room the brain makes available for growth and adaptation differs, often dramatically, from one person to the next. One can’t presume that duplicating Donald’s circumstances for others with autism would have the effect of duplicating his results.

Still, it’s clear that Donald reached his potential thanks, in large part, to the world he occupied—the world of Forest, Mississippi—and how it decided to respond to the odd child in its midst. Peter Gerhardt speaks of the importance of any community’s “acceptance” of those who have autism. In Forest, it appears, Donald was showered with acceptance, starting with the mother who defied experts to bring him back home, and continuing on to classmates from his childhood and golfing partners today. Donald’s neighbors not only shrug off his oddities, but openly admire his strengths—while taking a protective stance with any outsider whose intentions toward Donald may not have been sufficiently spelled out. On three occasions, while talking with townspeople who know Donald, we were advised, in strikingly similar language each time: “If what you’re doing hurts Don, I know where to find you.” We took the point: in Forest, Donald is “one of us.”

For a time, Donald’s care was literally shifted out into the community. Kanner believed that finding him a living situation in a more rural setting would be conducive to his development. So in 1942, the year he turned 9, Donald went to live with the Lewises, a farming couple who lived about 10 miles from town. His parents saw him frequently in this four-year period, and Kanner himself once traveled to Mississippi to observe the arrangement. He later said he was “amazed at the wisdom of the couple who took care of him.” The Lewises, who were childless, put Donald to work and made him useful. “They managed to give him [suitable] goals,” Kanner wrote in a later report.

    They made him use his preoccupation with measurements by having him dig a well and report on its depth … When he kept counting rows of corn over and over, they had him count the rows while plowing them. On my visit, he plowed six long rows; it was remarkable how well he handled the horse and plow and turned the horse around.

Kanner’s final observation on this visit speaks volumes about how Donald was perceived: “He attended a country school where his peculiarities were accepted and where he made good scholastic progress.”

Likewise, during high school, when Donald was again living back home with his parents, it appears his ways were mostly taken in stride. Janelle Brown, who was a few classes behind Donald (and the recipient of Donald Number 1,487), remembers that although he was teased a few times, he was generally regarded as a student who was enviably intelligent, even “brilliant”—again a legacy of his famous multiplication skills and brick-counting act. She recalls his sitting with a notebook and filling page after page with numbers, and her impression, as well as that of others, that they were seeing evidence of a superior mind at work.

It’s clear in all this that with the passage of time, Donald’s focus gradually turned outward. He increasingly came to terms with how his world was shaped, at the same time that his world was adjusting to him.

By 1957, he was a fraternity brother—Lambda Chi Alpha—at Millsaps College in Jackson, Mississippi, majoring in French and performing in the men’s a cappella choir. (The choir director, we were told by one member, never used a pitch pipe, because he took any note he needed directly from Donald.)

The Reverend Brister Ware, of the First Presbyterian Church in Jackson, was a fraternity brother and roommate of Donald’s. “He was a dear friend,” Ware says, recalling that he tried in various ways to give Donald a hand up socially, though “it was challenging to integrate him.” While training to be a water-safety instructor, he set out to teach Donald how to swim, “but the coordination was not so good for him.” Undaunted, Ware set another goal: “I thought I would try to open up his personality,” by introducing Donald to what was then a cool verbal affectation making the rounds, a way to pronounce the word yes as “yeeeeeeees.” Ware’s encouragements—to “put a little emotion and feeling and savoir faire into it”—again proved futile.

Ware was clearly rooting for his classmate, as were, he says, the other members of the fraternity. “I knew he was a little bit strange,” he admits. “But he’s genuine … I feel so lucky to have had him as a friend”—a friend, by the way, who gave Ware a number: 569.

Throughout Donald’s youth, it helped, no doubt, that the Tripletts had money—the money to get Leo Kanner’s attention in Baltimore, the funds to pay room and board at the Lewises’ farm. As the town’s bankers, they also had status, which may have discouraged the sort of cruelty that can come to people like Donald. One insightful resident of Forest put it this way: “In a small southern town, if you’re odd and poor, you’re crazy; if you’re odd and rich, all you are is a little eccentric.” When Donald was grown, the family bank employed him as a teller, and an irrevocable trust fund established by his family pays his bills to this day. The fund, according to his younger brother, Oliver, was designed with controls that ensure, as he put it, “some gal wouldn’t be able to talk Don into marrying her and then abscond.” In fact, Donald has never expressed any interest in girlfriends, nor has he had one.

But he has his brother—they dine together every Sunday, along with Oliver’s wife—and he has a community that has always accepted him, since long before people in town had heard the word autism. Tranquility, familiarity, stability, and security—if we were talking about healing, these would create an ideal environment. Forest provided all of them for Donald, who didn’t need to heal. He needed only to grow, and that he did, spectacularly. In one of her later letters to Leo Kanner, Mary Triplett reported: “He has taken his place in society very well, so much better than we ever hoped for.” There were still difficulties, of course—she confessed to the psychiatrist, by this time a friend, “I wish I knew what his inner feelings really are”—but her fears of having borne a “hopelessly insane child” were long past. By the time she died, Donald had grown into manhood, learning more about the world and his place in it than she could ever have imagined in those early years.

But he never could count bricks. This, it turns out, is a myth.

Donald explained how it had come about only after we’d been talking for some time. It had begun with a chance encounter more than 60 years ago outside his father’s law office, where some fellow high-school students, aware of his reputation as a math whiz, challenged him to count the bricks in the county courthouse across the street. Maybe they were picking on him a little; maybe they were just seeking entertainment. Regardless, Donald says he glanced quickly at the building and tossed out a large number at random. Apparently the other kids bought it on the spot, because the story would be told and retold over the years, with the setting eventually shifting from courthouse to school building—a captivating local legend never, apparently, fact-checked.

A common presumption is that people with autism are not good at telling fibs or spinning yarns, that they are too literal-minded to invent facts that don’t align with established reality. On one level, the story of Donald and the bricks demonstrates again the risks inherent in such pigeonholing. But on another level, it reveals something unexpected about Donald in particular. At the time of that episode, he was a teenager, barely a decade removed from the near-total social disconnect that had defined his early childhood. By adolescence, however, it seems he’d already begun working at connecting with people, and had grasped that his math skills were something that others admired.

We know that, because we finally asked him directly why he’d pulled that number out of the air all those years ago. He closed his eyes to answer, and then surprised us a final time. Speaking as abruptly as ever, and with the usual absence of detail, he said simply, and perhaps obviously, “I just wanted for those boys to think well of me.”
回复

使用道具 举报

3#
 楼主| 发表于 2010-10-29 10:55:45 | 只看该作者

re:http://gcontent.oeee...

http://gcontent.oeeee.com/9/af/9af76329c78e28c9/Blog/c99/048eb5.html

可爱的《南方都市报》已经把上面的报道翻译出来了~~

自闭症第一人
类别:国际观察   浏览量:
374
   版次:GB07   版名:地球周刊 人物   稿源:南方都市报   2010-10-10
作者:Dawn 原创

摘要:1943年,10岁的唐纳德被诊断患了一种前所未有的疾病———自闭症。现在77岁的他过得如何?在自闭症患者激增的今天,他的经历能给我们带来何种启示?

1943年,10岁的唐纳德被诊断患了一种前所未有的疾病———自闭症。现在77岁的他过得如何?在自闭症患者激增的今天,他的经历能给我们带来何种启示?

    1951年,据说懂读心术的催眠士弗兰兹·波尔加应邀到密西西比州的弗雷斯特作表演。这个小镇仅有3000居民,没有旅店,波尔加被安排在当地一家富户借宿。

    之前几年,波尔加走遍美国,神奇的催眠术为他博得了巨大名声。但留宿弗雷斯特这一夜最令他难忘,因为这晚他遇到了主人家的大儿子、18岁的唐纳德。这孩子出奇地冷漠,动作笨拙,却又有神奇的本领,比如在别人弹琴时丝毫不差地唱出音符,还有天才的心算能力。波尔加说“87乘23”,话音刚落,他就给出答案 “2001”。

    事实上,唐纳德早已成为本地一个传说。附近居民都知道弗雷斯特有个孩子,只看一眼学校砖墙,就知道它用了多少块砖。

    表演结束后,波尔加找到房东夫妇,请求他们允许他把唐纳德带走,和他一起表演。

    唐纳德的父母大吃一惊。“我妈妈一点兴趣也没有,”唐纳德的弟弟奥利弗回忆说,“她向波尔加解释说我哥哥还在上学,不能抛下功课一走了之。”

    虽然没有说出来,唐纳德的父母却无疑有种受辱感。唐纳德与众不同,这一点他们无法否认,但他们不希望唐纳德被当成稀罕物来看,于是坚决地拒绝了波尔加的要求。

    全知的催眠士波尔加可能不知道,唐纳德虽然错过了与他一同站在聚光灯下的机会,却已经在历史上占有一席之地。他的名字被写入诸多论著,传遍整个世界,很快就变得比波尔加更为有名。

    这是因为唐纳德是首个被诊断患有自闭症的孩子。在自闭症文献中,他被称为“第一病例”,简称“Donald T”。1943年一篇文章首先提到他,该文宣布发现了“一种前所未有的疾病”,“一种复杂的神经紊乱症”(如今统称为“自闭症谱系障碍”,简称ASD)。

    那是67年前的事。当时,人们认为这种病极其罕见。而现在,医生、家长和政治家们不时谈到自闭症的“蔓延”。自1990年代早期以来,A SD患者激增,平均每110个孩子中就有1个表现出自闭症症状,没人知道为什么。

    关于自闭症的起因,一直众说纷纭。早期,精神病学认为自闭症是“坏妈妈”导致的,后来人们发现自闭症亦有生物学基础,但这带来了更多混乱。有人认为罪魁是食物中的麸质,也有人说某些疫苗防腐剂中含有水银,会导致自闭症,还有一些学派把自闭症视为自体免疫反应,或是营养不良的结果。不过,关于自闭症的主要症状,争议不多。《精神疾病诊断与统计手册》给出了清晰的描述,1930年代“Donald T”到约翰霍普金斯大学首次接受检查时,就完全吻合这些症状。

    此后数年间,科学文献几次对Donald T的病情发展作了补充。但是从大约40年前开始,叙述中断,他生命中后来的章节无人书写。如果你想知道唐纳德如今的生活,几乎找不到答案。于是我们决定亲自去寻找唐纳德,他今年77岁了,依然住在弗雷斯特。(南方都市报  www.nddaily.com SouthernMetropolisDailyMark 南都网)

    自闭症儿童的父母心头时常萦绕着这样一个问题:我死之后怎么办?自闭症儿童总是要成长为大人,而且多数情况下会比照顾他们的父母活得更久。那么,父母死后他们怎么办?这个问题还没有引起重视,人们目前更关注自闭症对童年的影响。但今日自闭症儿童的激增就意味着明天自闭症成人的激增,数字摆在眼前:10年后美国50多万自闭症儿童将步入成年。

    我们将如何面对这50多万人?有人主张将他们隔离,同时满足其基本生活需要——— 过去我们曾经这样对待残障人士;或者,我们可采取正常化的心态,把自闭症视为人类这块纺织品上又一个自然的皱折,承认他们是我们的一部分。

    “自闭症第一人”唐纳德·崔普就生活在这两种观点交织的世界里。

    现在是黄昏时分,唐纳德正开着他那辆咖啡色的卡迪拉克,沿着密西西比州80号公路向南。他开车时有一种轻微的点击动作:踩下油门一秒后短暂抬脚,然后再踩,踩,抬,踩,抬……一直重复,节奏从不改变。

    这一天他的日程表排得满满的:早上跟朋友喝咖啡,然后散步,回家看电视剧《大淘金》,这会儿要去打高尔夫。“我注意到你的车上贴着一张拉法耶特县的标签,” 他打破了沉默。过了一会,又说:“这意味着它来自拉法耶特县。”然后他再度陷入沉默,注意力转向眼前的公路,偶尔自言自语———鉴于他和人交谈时有闭眼的习惯,不说话可能是最安全的选择。

    他将车停在弗雷斯特俱乐部前。这是一栋红砖小楼,会籍以家庭为单位,每月100美元,来打球的有律师、银行家,也有卡车司机、推销员和农民。他们并不都知道“D T”———唐纳德的昵称———有自闭症,但当他走向球座时,人人都会注意到那古怪的动作:胳膊呈A字形甩动,步态有些机械,脑袋和肩膀就像节拍器一样,以左-右-左的节律摆动。

    唐纳德球打得不错,能轻松搞定六英尺的果岭推杆。但他挥杆却像演哑剧,每次都要重复一系列仪式性的动作,尤其在他很想进球的时候。

    唐纳德一般都是一个人打。但他有时别无选择,只能跟别人合作——— 俱尔部举行传统的争夺赛时,会把球手们随机组队。在最近一次比赛中,唐纳德与其他四人合作,他打得很好,也会跟伙伴们开玩笑,只是句式往往是重复的,他还会以独特方式,将伙伴们的名字进行组合,“嗨,玛丽切丽!”“谢谢,肯尼斯塞尼斯!”

    不过,多数时候,唐纳德是沉默的。当然,这跟高尔夫球场的礼仪相符。唐纳德满足于沉默,满足于现在的生活。他自由、独立、身体健康。一句话,“自闭症第一人”过得非常好。

    这在当初不可想像。唐纳德3岁时就被送进了专门机构。因为拒绝吃饭,还有父母处理不了的其他问题,医生建议给他“改变环境”,1937年8月,唐纳德被送进距家50英里的一家疗养院。

    这家疗养院的功能在今天看来很奇怪:隔离那些可能会得肺结核的孩子。因此,它不是为唐纳德这样的孩子设计的,根据记录,他去之后反应很强烈,身体“迅速衰弱”。

    当时,把患有精神疾病的孩子送进专门机构是缺省选择,连他妈妈也认为唐纳德得了精神病,她在一封信中称他为“我那没有希望的精神病孩子”。然而,疗养院并没有什么帮助。约翰霍普金斯大学评估认为,“他在那里度过了最糟糕的时光”。父母每月只能探视两次,疗养院禁止他接触其他一切———人、玩具、食物、音乐、游戏,每天他就“一动不动地坐着,对一切都不关心”。

    1938年8月,唐纳德的父母来接他。疗养院负责人告诉他们,孩子恢复得很好,让他们不要把孩子接回家———原话是“随他去”。

    但他们坚持把唐纳德带回家。为唐纳德在院表现写评估时,院长很不上心,唐纳德住了一年,他只写了半页,说这孩子可能是患有“某种腺体疾病”。

    于是,即将5岁的唐纳德又回到了起点。

    心理学博士彼得·吉哈德右手挥舞着一张V isa卡,演示如何正确使用它。他的拇指完全盖住右下角的蓝白标志,“这样塞进读卡器,方向永远不会错”。

    吉哈德不是在展示生活小技巧,而是在告诉我们如何让自闭症人群被其他人接受。

    在现实生活中,我们通常不认为患自闭症的成年人和自闭症儿童一样需要帮助。我们只觉得他们古怪,对他们感到不耐烦。当你在超市排长队,看到前面那家伙迟迟不能正确地刷卡,一般说不出好话。吉哈德说,此时刷卡技巧就成为社会生存问题。如果自闭症患者能顺利解决这个问题,就向正常社会迈出了一大步。

    吉哈德是美国研究自闭症成人的头号专家。“人们都不想研究这个,所以我独享殊荣。”他说。自闭症儿童研究是热点,研究成人“没有吸引力”。成人代表着更大挑战,“他们个头大,发脾气时可以实施真正的攻击;他们有性欲;很多能让孩子高兴、能分散其注意力的招数在成人那儿不起作用”。(南方都市报  www.nddaily.com SouthernMetropolisDailyMark 南都网)

    “人们倾向于把成年患者当作大号儿童对待,”吉哈德说,“这样不行,他们是成人。”和我们一样,他们渴望“体验生活”。“比如交友,做有趣的工作,拥有想要的东西,我们认为有价值的东西,他们一样珍视。”

    吉哈德认为,要让自闭症成人独立生存,首要的是教会他们必要的生活技巧:理解钱的功能;请求指导,然后遵从;穿干净的衣服;坐公交车,识别危险人群;还有———可能是最困难的———找工作时直视面试者。

    这又带来一个问题:自闭症成人应在哪里生活?现实中,85%的自闭症成人跟父母、兄弟姐妹或其他亲友一起住。但当没有这些选择的时候,该怎么办?一个替代办法是在社区中替他们安排集体住所,让他们在家庭般的环境中得到帮助。然而,鉴于启动成本和邻居们的抵制,这样的地方有限,全美有超过8.8万名患者正排队等候。所有这些,都让那些自问“我死之后怎么办”的父母很不安。

    如果不是父母竭力为唐纳德寻找最好的生活,他的名字很可能不会进入医学史。他的母亲玛丽生于迈克克拉维家族,那是金融之家,至今控制着弗雷斯特银行。和当时多数女性不同,她上了大学,然后嫁给了前镇长的儿子、律师奥利弗·崔普莱特。崔普莱特毕业于耶鲁法学院,开了一个律师事务所,后来成为美国最高法院律师。他是个敏感的人,曾两次神经失常,有时一路回来,会不记得路上见过的任何人任何事。但作为律师,人们认为他很棒,因此向玛丽提亲时,她们家并未反对。

    唐纳德出生于1933年9月。5年后他在疗养院时弟弟出生,跟爸爸一样也叫奥利弗。1938年10月,崔普莱特夫妇将幼子交给家中老人,带着唐纳德坐上去巴尔的摩的火车,找美国当时顶尖的心理学家、约翰霍普金斯大学教授里奥·康纳(他撰写的《儿童精神病学》1935年一出版即成为医学院教科书,一再重印)求诊。

    会面前,奥利弗给康纳写过一封长达33页的信,细致地描述了唐纳德的症状。如今这封信在自闭症研究中占据着独特地位,数十年来被翻译成多种语言。

    奥利弗写道,小家伙几乎从未哭着找妈妈。他似乎“缩到一个壳里”,“住在自己的世界”,“对周围一切都漠不关心。”虽然对人类完全不感兴趣———包括父母 ——— 他仍有几种癖好,包括“极度喜欢旋转积木,喜欢平底锅和其他圆形物体”。他对数字、音符、美国总统的照片和字母表里的字母极为着迷,喜欢倒着念这些字母。他动作笨拙,对某些东西特别讨厌:牛奶、秋千、三轮车———“几乎有种恐惧”。他不喜欢日常生活有任何变化或者思绪被打断,“否则会发怒,表现出极强的破坏性”。叫他的名字基本没反应,似乎没听见,去哪儿都要人领着或带着去。问他问题,要么不回答,要么只用一个词。他会被一些词句迷住,不停地绕着念。

    与此同时,唐纳德又表现出一些惊人的天赋。2岁就能背出《圣歌第二十三章》和长老会讲道集。他旋转积木时随意发出的哼哼声原来一点也不随意,用他哼的音符和节奏在琴上可以弹出完美的和弦。一个人待着时,唐纳德显得十分聪明。“他好像一直在思考,思考,”他的父亲写道,“他独处时最幸福。”

    见到唐纳德后,康纳发现的确如此,甚至更甚。他回忆说,唐纳德进屋后,径直奔向积木和玩具,“根本没注意到人的存在”。康纳用针戳了唐纳德一下,发现他不喜欢这样——— 很痛——— 但他并未因此对康纳表现出丝毫戒备和厌恶,他似乎不能把痛苦和制造这种痛苦的人联系起来。事实上,整个见面过程中,唐纳德完全无视康纳,仿佛他和桌子、书架是一类的东西。

    在这次会面的记录中,有一个打了问号的诊断:精神分裂症。这是当时最有可能的一个判断,因为唐纳德显然非常聪明,精神分裂症患者常会这样。但是其举止显示,他没有精神分裂症患者常有的幻觉。

    康纳继续观察了唐纳德两周,不知道该如何诊断。后来崔普莱特一家又三赴巴尔的摩,都没有结论。1942年9月,首次见到唐纳德4年后,康纳写信给玛丽,说他渐渐意识到自己“正看到一种前所未有的新病”。“我手头积累了另外八个病例,跟唐很像。”他写道。

    他将唐纳德和其他八个孩子的症状总结了一下:对人缺乏兴趣,对物极为迷恋,喜欢千篇一律,喜欢独处。“如果非得给这种症状取个名字,我觉得应该是‘专注自我而与现实隔绝的孤独症’。”

    孤独症(autistic)这个名词不是康纳发明的,精神病学早已使用它来描述一些精神分裂症患者怯于与人交流的状态。和歇斯底里一样,它描述的是一种症状,而非疾病。但是现在康纳用它来定义一系列复杂的行为,形成一种独特的、从未经过验证的诊断:自闭症。

    1943年,康纳在期刊《神经症儿童》上披露了这一发现,共列举了11个病例,唐纳德排在首位。

    几十年间,唐纳德学会了许多技能 — ——比 如 开 车 和 打 高 尔夫——— 但他一直回避与人对话。他有时会主动提问,但一般是为了得到信息(“现在几点了?”)或者表述偶然的发现(比如提到我的车上有个标签),正常的你来我往的对话他似乎从未经历过。别人提问时,他只会简洁、单向地作答,仿佛在做试卷。而且,他喜欢使用数字,比如日期。他惯于将遇到的人编码。他一位老友巴迪·勒维特说,1950年代末,唐纳德分配给他的号码是333。他跟我们说,下次见到唐纳德直接问他的编号是多少,虽然时隔多年,唐纳德肯定记得。

    果然,第二天我们问及此事,还没讲完,唐纳德就说出了333。我们做了几次类似测试,把多年来曾被编码的人列出来,唐纳德都能准确给出他们的编码。当然,那些被编码的人也都记得自己的号码,他们觉得这是一种荣誉。

    唐纳德肯定不这么想。荣誉这种抽象说法是自闭症患者不太可能理解的概念之一,他们在由既定事实排列而成的世界里更加愉快。他们是按字面意义理解事物,因此人们普遍相信自闭症患者很难撒谎,或者很难欣赏笑话。尽管唐纳德非常享受罗列人物、地点和事件,他却不太容易领会暗示、别人的语气或者情绪。照顾他多年的父母去世时,他都没有悲伤的表现。

    彼得·吉哈德讲了55岁的自闭症患者托尼的故事。托尼一直跟母亲生活,然后她去世了。根据以往参加葬礼的经验,他知道自己作为“丧家”,将要和前来吊唁的人拥抱。葬礼上他观察弟弟的做法,跟着模仿,处理得很好。

    第二天,住在隔壁的女士——— 不是托尼家密友,只是邻居——— 带着食物前来表示安慰,托尼对她表示感谢,而她也表达了哀悼之情。

    吉哈德说,接下来发生的一切堪称自闭症患者行为教科书。“托尼想,好,她表示了哀悼,那么下面我该拥抱她了。于是他就过去,抱住了她。”吉哈德说,那位女士的确挣扎过,暗示她不想被抱,但是托尼理解不了这些信号:“他抱住了她,可能有些笨拙——— 时间有点长,用力有点大——— 结果她报了警,说受到了性骚扰。”

    在吉哈德看来,这仿佛普通人与自闭症患者交流的一个隐喻。托尼很聪明,读完了大学,但他却无法判断一个人是否希望被拥抱。他知道人们会有一些暗示,但不知道哪些是。他后来告诉吉哈德:“那些规则好像千变万化,我刚学会一种规则,下一个人的方式又不一样了。”

    吉哈德认为,要想解决这个问题,必须对自闭症患者做出正确的指导。学校在教育自闭症儿童时,过于强调学业,却没有教给他们真正需要的东西,即一系列社交技巧,比如避免错误的拥抱。当他们成为大人,受教育的机会就更少了。

    “拥抱事件”还凸显了另外一个问题:邻居对于自闭症患者缺乏认识。如果她对托尼的疾病有所了解,知道它会表现为何种症状,就不会如此惊慌失措。至少她可以直接告诉托尼希望他放开,而不是进行徒劳的暗示。

    现在唐纳德独自生活在父母留下的老房子里。周围环境优美,但房子需要刷新,好几个房间———如客厅和饭厅——— 都黑黑的,因太久不用散发出霉味。唐纳德很少进入这些地方,对他来说,厨房、浴室和卧室已经足够。

    但是,一月一次,唐纳德会离开房子和小镇。

    他热爱旅行。作为自闭症患者,这一点很特别。他去过36个国家和美国28个州,埃及去了3次,伊斯坦布尔5次,夏威夷17次。他去过非洲狩猎,出过几次海,还参加过无数次高尔夫球协会锦标赛。

    不过,他并非旅行癖。多数时候他最多出去六天,事后也不会跟途中遇到的人联系。他的目标是把图片中看到的地方亲自拍下来,收集在相册中,然后就开始计划下一次出行,如果是国内就自己订航班,国外的话就依靠杰克逊市一家代理。他可能是弗雷斯顿旅行最多的人。

    你很难想像,这是那个小时候只爱转动积木、自己转圈,或者反复念叨一些单词的孩子。当时看来,他似乎注定要过单调、狭隘的生活,在疗养院的窗后面呆呆度过余生。结果完全不同,他分别在23岁、27岁和36岁学会了打球、开车、环游世界,长大后的唐纳德仍在继续发展。

    自闭症是一种非常个人化的病症,患者大脑的成长空间和适应性各有不同,有时差别颇大,在另外一位自闭者身上未必能复制唐纳德的成功。但是显然,唐纳德能充分发挥潜力,很大部分要归功于他所在的友好环境。吉哈德说,社区对自闭者的接纳非常重要。弗雷斯特人对唐纳德的接纳无处不在。先是妈妈不顾专家反对,把他带回家,然后是同学和球友的友好,邻居们不仅对他的古怪不以为意,还十分欣赏他的优点,注意保护他。在和当地人谈到唐纳德时,他们都对我们发出类似的警告:“如果你们伤害了唐,我知道到哪儿去找你。”我们明白了:在弗雷斯特,唐纳德是“我们中的一分子”。

    事实上,曾经一度,照顾唐纳德的任务真的落到了别人头上。考纳认为乡村生活对他的身心发展有益,因此1942年,唐纳德9岁时,被送到距弗雷斯特约10英里的农民勒维斯家寄宿。四年中父母经常去看他,考纳也曾亲自到密西西比来看事情进展如何,他很为勒维斯夫妇的智慧而惊讶。没有孩子的勒维斯夫妇让唐纳德参与一些工作,让他变成一个有用的人。“他们会为他设定合适的目标,”考纳在论文中写道,“比如让他挖一口小井,报告其深度,让他一边种玉米一边数垄数,满足他对数字的爱好。我到访时,他种了六垄,看到他那么自如地赶着马转弯,我不禁惊叹。”

    同样,回到父母身边读高中时,唐纳德也得到了广泛的认可。同学视唐纳德为天才,津津乐道他的速算和数砖故事。在杰克逊上大学时,他加入了兄弟会和男子合唱团,据说因为有了唐纳德,合唱团指挥从未用过定音器。室友沃尔努力教他学游泳,鼓励他玩说唱,“玩弄字词是他的强项”。

    崔普莱特家很有钱,在唐纳德的青少年时期,这一点无疑很重要。有钱才能去巴尔的摩看专家,才能付得起他在勒维斯夫妇家的寄宿费。作为银行家,他们也有地位,这可能减少了人们对唐纳德的轻视。弗雷斯特一位居民说:“在一个南部小城镇,如果你又怪又穷,那就是个疯子;如果你有点怪但有钱,那只是有点不合时宜。”唐纳德长大后,家族银行雇佣他当出纳员,还设立了一个信托基金,支付他的账单。弟弟奥利弗说,这个基金经过精心设计,可以避免一些女孩骗唐结婚后拿钱跑掉。

    不过唐纳德从未对交女友发生过兴趣。他有弟弟一家,有接纳他的社区,这就够了。弗雷斯特镇为唐纳德提供了平静、亲密和安全的环境。他不需要康复,只需要成长,他令人惊叹地做到了这一点。在后来给考纳的一封信中,玛丽写道:“他很好地在社会上立足,比我们期待的好多了。”

    但是他不再会数砖头了,这一点似乎是个谜。(南方都市报  www.nddaily.com SouthernMetropolisDailyMark 南都网)

    我们接触一段后,唐纳德才谈到这件事。那是60多年前,在他父亲的事务所外,当时一些高中生听说他是数学怪才,就发起挑战,让他数街对面县法院大楼所用的砖头。唐纳德瞄了那栋建筑一眼,随口说出了一个巨大的数字,显然那些孩子相信了他的说法。多年来这个故事一再被转述,只是地点从法院变成了学校,成了一个从未被验证的传奇。

    关于自闭症患者,人们有一个共识,那就是他们不善于撒谎或者编故事,因为他们的思维太实事求是,想像力缺乏,无法发明与既定事实不符的故事。但唐纳德数砖的故事再次显示了成见的局限性,提示了唐纳德的特别之处。数砖时他只有十来岁,刚刚脱离与世隔绝的童年不久,然而已经学会与人打交道,并知道数学才能是自己的亮点。

    我们知道这一点,是因为我们最后直接问唐纳德,为何当时要随意说出一个数字,他闭上眼睛,给了我们一个惊人的答案:“我只是想让那些孩子对我有个好印象。”

    编译:Dawn
回复

使用道具 举报

本版积分规则

小黑屋|手机版|Archiver|以琳自闭症论坛

GMT+8, 2024-11-28 12:01

Powered by Discuz! X3.2

© 2001-2013 Comsenz Inc.

快速回复 返回顶部 返回列表